Tuesday, May 29, 2007
Saturday, May 26, 2007
Great videoclip about global uniformization operated by the states
Rammstein new videoclip called Amerika is just great. It contains many archetypal elements as well as nice allusions to the Moon landing controversy. Who said rammstein were stupid ? These guys are damn smart I would say.
Friday, May 25, 2007
Monday, May 21, 2007
Out-of-body experiences may be caused by arousal system disturbances in brain
Out-of-body experiences may be caused by arousal system disturbances in brain from PhysOrg.com
Having an out-of-body experience may seem far-fetched to some, but for those with arousal system disturbances in their brains, it may not be a far off idea that they could sense they were really outside their own body watching themselves. In previous studies of more than 13,000 Europeans, almost 6 percent said they have had such an out-of-body experience.
[...]
The Psychedelic Experience Movie
Monday, May 14, 2007
Mirror Neurons and imitation learning as the driving force behind "the great leap forward" in human evolution, TMS and bio-electricity in neurons
The discovery of mirror neurons in the frontal lobes of monkeys, and their potential relevance to human brain evolution — which I speculate on in this essay — is the single most important "unreported" (or at least, unpublicized) story of the decade. I predict that mirror neurons will do for psychology what DNA did for biology: they will provide a unifying framework and help explain a host of mental abilities that have hitherto remained mysterious and inaccessible to experiments.
There are many puzzling questions about the evolution of the human mind and brain:
1) The hominid brain reached almost its present size — and perhaps even its present intellectual capacity about 250,000 years ago . Yet many of the attributes we regard as uniquely human appeared only much later. Why? What was the brain doing during the long "incubation "period? Why did it have all this latent potential for tool use, fire, art music and perhaps even language- that blossomed only considerably later? How did these latent abilities emerge, given that natural selection can only select expressed abilities, not latent ones? I shall call this "Wallace's problem", after the Victorian naturalist Alfred Russell Wallace who first proposed it.
2) Crude "Oldawan" tools — made by just a few blows to a core stone to create an irregular edge — emerged 2.4 million ago and were probably made by Homo Habilis whose brain size was half way (700cc) between modern humans (1300) and chimps (400). After another million years of evolutionary stasis aesthetically pleasing "symmetrical" tools began to appear associated with a standardization of production technique and artifact form. These required switching from a hard hammer to a soft (wooden?) hammer while the tool was being made, in order to ensure a smooth rather than jagged, irregular edge. And lastly, the invention of stereotyped "assembly line" tools (sophisticated symmetrical bifacial tools) that were hafted to a handle, took place only 200,000 years ago. Why was the evolution of the human mind "punctuated" by these relatively sudden upheavals of technological change?
3) Why the sudden explosion (often called the "great leap" ) in technological sophistication, widespread cave art, clothes, stereotyped dwellings, etc. around 40 thousand years ago, even though the brain had achieved its present "modern" size almost a million years earlier?
4) Did language appear completely out of the blue as suggested by Chomsky? Or did it evolve from a more primitive gestural language that was already in place?
5) Humans are often called the "Machiavellian Primate" referring to our ability to "read minds" in order to predict other peoples' behavior and outsmart them. Why are apes and humans so good at reading other individuals' intentions? Do higher primates have a specialized brain center or module for generating a "theory of other minds" as proposed by Nick Humphrey and Simon Baron-Cohen? If so, where is this circuit and how and when did it evolve?
The solution to many of these riddles comes from an unlikely source.. the study of single neurons in the brains of monkeys. I suggest that the questions become less puzzling when you consider Giaccamo Rizzollati's recent discovery of "mirror neurons' in the ventral premotor area of monkeys. This cluster of neurons, I argue, holds the key to understanding many enigmatic aspects of human evolution. Rizzollati and Arbib have already pointed out the relevance of their discovery to language evolution . But I believe the significance of their findings for understanding other equally important aspects of human evolution has been largely overlooked. This, in my view, is the most important unreported "story" in the last decade.
THE EMERGENCE OF LANGUAGE
Unlike many other human traits such as humor, art, dancing or music the survival value of language is obvious — it helps us communicate our thoughts and intentions. But the question of how such an extraordinary ability might have actually evolved has puzzled biologists, psychologists and philosophers at least since the time of Charles Darwin. The problem is that the human vocal apparatus is vastly more sophisticated than that of any ape but without the correspondingly sophisticated language areas in the brain the vocal equipment alone would be useless. So how did these two mechanisms with so many sophisticated interlocking parts evolve in tandem? Following Darwin's lead I suggest that our vocal equipment and our remarkable ability to modulate voice evolved mainly for producing emotional calls and musical sounds during courtship ("croonin a toon."). Once that evolved then the brain — especially the left hemisphere — could evolve language.
But a bigger puzzle remains. Is language mediated by a sophisticated and highly specialized "language organ" that is unique to humans and emerged completely out of the blue as suggested by Chomsky? Or was there a more primitive gestural communication system already in place that provided a scaffolding for the emergence of vocal language?
Rizzolatti's discovery can help us solve this age-old puzzle. He recorded from the ventral premotor area of the frontal lobes of monkeys and found that certain cells will fire when a monkey performs a single, highly specific action with its hand: pulling, pushing, tugging, grasping, picking up and putting a peanut in the mouth etc. different neurons fire in response to different actions. One might be tempted to think that these are motor "command" neurons, making muscles do certain things; however, the astonishing truth is that any given mirror neuron will also fire when the monkey in question observes another monkey (or even the experimenter) performing the same action, e.g. tasting a peanut!
With knowledge of these neurons, you have the basis for understanding a host of very enigmatic aspects of the human mind: "mind reading" empathy, imitation learning, and even the evolution of language. Anytime you watch someone else doing something (or even starting to do something), the corresponding mirror neuron might fire in your brain, thereby allowing you to "read" and understand another's intentions, and thus to develop a sophisticated "theory of other minds." (I suggest, also, that a loss of these mirror neurons may explain autism — a cruel disease that afflicts children. Without these neurons the child can no longer understand or empathize with other people emotionally and therefore completely withdraws from the world socially.)
Mirror neurons can also enable you to imitate the movements of others thereby setting the stage for the complex Lamarckian or cultural inheritance that characterizes our species and liberates us from the constraints of a purely gene based evolution. Moreover, as Rizzolati has noted, these neurons may also enable you to mime — and possibly understand — the lip and tongue movements of others which, in turn, could provide the opportunity for language to evolve. (This is why, when you stick your tongue out at a new born baby it will reciprocate! How ironic and poignant that this little gesture encapsulates a half a million years of primate brain evolution.) Once you have these two abilities in place the ability to read someone's intentions and the ability to mime their vocalizations then you have set in motion the evolution of language. You need no longer speak of a unique language organ and the problem doesn't seem quite so mysterious any more.
(Another important piece of the puzzle is Rizzolatti's observation that the ventral premotor area may be a homologue of the "Broca's area" — a brain center associated with the expressive and syntactic aspects of language in humans).
These arguments do not in any way negate the idea that there are specialized brain areas for language in humans. We are dealing, here, with the question of how such areas may have evolved, not whether they exist or not.
Mirror neurons were discovered in monkeys but how do we know they exist in the human brain? To find out we studied patients with a strange disorder called anosognosia. Most patients with a right hemisphere stroke have complete paralysis of the left side of their body and will complain about it, as expected. But about 5% of them will vehemently deny their paralysis even though they are mentally otherwise lucid and intelligent. This is the so called "denial" syndrome or anosognosia.
To our amazement, we found that some of these patients not only denied their own paralysis, but also denied the paralysis of another patient whose inability to move his arm was clearly visible to them and to others. Denying ones one paralysis is odd enough but why would a patient deny another patient's paralysis? We suggest that this bizarre observation is best understood in terms of damage to Rizzolatti's mirror neurons. It's as if anytime you want to make a judgement about someone else's movements you have to run a VR (virtual reality) simulation of the corresponding movements in your own brain and without mirror neurons you cannot do this .
The second piece of evidence comes from studying brain waves (EEG) in humans. When people move their hands a brain wave called the MU wave gets blocked and disappears completely. Eric Altschuller, Jamie Pineda, and I suggested at the Society for Neurosciences in 1998 that this suppression was caused by Rizzolati's mirror neuron system. Consistent with this theory we found that such a suppression also occurs when a person watches someone else moving his hand but not if he watches a similar movement by an inanimate object. (We predict that children with autism should show suppression if they move their own hands but not if they watch some one else. Our lab now has preliminary hints from one highly functioning autistic child that this might be true (Social Neuroscience Abstracts 2000).
THE BIG BANG OF HUMAN EVOLUTION
The hominid brain grew at an accelerating pace until it reached its present size of 1500cc about 200,000 years ago. Yet uniquely human abilities such the invention of highly sophisticated "standardized" multi- part tools, tailored clothes, art, religious belief and perhaps even language are thought to have emerged quite rapidly around 40,000 years ago — a sudden explosion of human mental abilities and culture that is sometimes called the "big bang." If the brain reached its full human potential — or at least size — 200,000 years ago why did it remain idle for 150,000 years?
Most scholars are convinced that the big bang occurred because of some unknown genetic change in brain structure. For instance, the archeologist Steve Mithen has just written a book in which he claims that before the big bang there were three different brain modules in the human brain that were specialized for "social or machiavellian intelligence", for "mechanical intelligence" or tool use, and for "natural history" (a propensity to classify). These three modules remained isolated from each other but around 50,000 years ago some genetic change in the brain suddenly allowed them to communicate with each other, resulting in the enormous flexibility and versatility of human consciousness.
I disagree with Mithen ingenious suggestion and offer a very different solution to the problem. (This is not incompatible with Mithen's view but its a different idea). I suggest that the so-called big bang occurred because certain critical environmental triggers acted on a brain that had already become big for some other reason and was therefore "pre-adapted" for those cultural innovations that make us uniquely human. (One of the key pre adaptations being mirror neurons.)
Inventions like tool use, art, math and even aspects of language may have been invented "accidentally" in one place and then spread very quickly given the human brain's amazing capacity for imitation learning and mind reading using mirror neurons. Perhaps ANY major "innovation" happens because of a fortuitous coincidence of environmental circumstances — usually at a single place and time. But given our species' remarkable propensity for miming, such an invention would tend to spread very quickly through the population — once it emerged.
Mirror neurons obviously cannot be the only answer to all these riddles of evolution. After all rhesus monkeys and apes have them, yet they lack the cultural sophistication of humans (although it has recently been shown that chimps at least DO have the rudiments of culture, even in the wild). I would argue, though, that mirror neurons are Necessary but not sufficient: their emergence and further development in hominids was a decisive step. The reason is that once you have a certain minimum amount of "imitation learning" and "culture" in place, this culture can, in turn, exert the selection pressure for developing those additional mental traits that make us human . And once this starts happening you have set in motion the auto-catalytic process that culminated in modern human consciousness.
A second problem with my suggestion is that it doesn't explain why the many human innovations that constitute the big bang occurred during a relatively short period. If its simply a matter of chance discoveries spreading rapidly,why would all of them have occurred at the same time? There are three answers to this objection. First,the evidence that it all took place at the same time is tenuous. The invention of music, shelters,hafted tools, tailored clothing, writing, speech, etc. may have been spread out between 100K and 5k and the so-called great leap may be a sampling artifact of archeological excavation. Second, any given innovation (e.g. speech or writing or tools) may have served as a catalyst for the others and may have therefore accelerated the pace of culture as a whole. And third, there may indeed have been a genetic change,b ut it may not have been an increase in the ability to innovate ( nor a breakdown of barriers between modules as suggested by Mithen) but an increase in the sophistication of the mirror neuron system and therefore in "learnability."
The resulting increase in ability to imitate and learn (and teach) would then explain the explosion of cultural change that we call the "great leap forward" or the "big bang" in human evolution. This argument implies that the whole "nature-nurture debate" is largely meaningless as far as human are concerned. Without the genetically specified learnability that characterizes the human brain Homo sapiens wouldn't deserve the title "sapiens" (wise) but without being immersed in a culture that can take advantage of this learnability, the title would be equally inappropriate. In this sense human culture and human brain have co-evolved into obligatory mutual parasites — without either the result would not be a human being. (No more than you can have a cell without its parasitic mitochondria).
THE SECOND BIG BANG
My suggestion that these neurons provided the initial impetus for "runaway" brain/ culture co-evolution in humans, isn't quite as bizarre as it sounds. Imagine a martian anthropologist was studying human evolution a million years from now. He would be puzzled (like Wallace was) by the relatively sudden emergence of certain mental traits like sophisticated tool use, use of fire, art and "culture" and would try to correlate them (as many anthropologists now do) with purported changes in brain size and anatomy caused by mutations. But unlike them he would also be puzzled by the enormous upheavals and changes that occurred after (say) 19th century — what we call the scientific/industrial revolution. This revolution is, in many ways, much more dramatic (e.g. the sudden emergence of nuclear power, automobiles, air travel, and space travel) than the "great leap forward" that happened 40,000 years ago!!
He might be tempted to argue that there must have been a genetic change and corresponding change in brain anatomy and behavior to account for this second leap forward. (Just as many anthropologists today seek a genetic explanation for the first one.) Yet we know that present one occurred exclusively because of fortuitous environmental circumstances, because Galileo invented the "experimental method," that, together with royal patronage and the invention of the printing press, kicked off the scientific revolution. His experiments and the earlier invention of a sophisticated new language called mathematics in India in the first millennium AD (based on place value notation, zero and the decimal system), set the stage for Newtonian mechanics and the calculus and "the rest is history" as we say.
Now the thing to bear in mind is that none of this need have happened. It certainly did not happen because of a genetic change in the human brains during the renaissance. It happened at least partly because of imitation learning and rapid "cultural" transmission of knowledge. (Indeed one could almost argue that there was a greater behavioral/cognitive difference between pre-18th century and post 20th century humans than between Homo Erectus and archaic Homo Sapiens. Unless he knew better our Martian ethologist may conclude that there was a bigger genetic difference between the first two groups than the latter two species!)
Based on this analogy I suggest, further, that even the first great leap forward was made possible largely by imitation and emulation. Wallace's question was perfectly sensible; it is very puzzling how a set of extraordinary abilities seemed to emerge "out of the blue". But his solution was wrong...the apparently sudden emergence of things like art or sophisticated tools was not because of God or "divine intervention". I would argue instead that just as a single invention (or two) by Galileo and Gutenberg quickly spread and transformed the surface of the globe (although there was no preceding genetic change), inventions like fire, tailored clothes, "symmetrical tools", and art, etc. may have fortuitously emerged in a single place and then spread very quickly.
Such inventions may have been made by earlier hominids too (even chimps and orangs are remarkably inventive...who knows how inventive Homo Erectus or Neandertals were) but early hominids simply may not have had an advanced enough mirror neuron system to allow a rapid transmission and dissemination of ideas. So the ideas quickly drop out of the "meme pool". This system of cells, once it became sophisticated enough to be harnessed for "training" in tool use and for reading other hominids minds, may have played the same pivotal role in the emergence of human consciousness (and replacement of Neandertals by Homo Sapiens) as the asteroid impact did in the triumph of mammals over reptiles.
So it makes no more sense to ask "Why did sophisticated tool use and art emerge only 40,000 years ago even though the brain had all the required latent ability 100,000 years earlier?" — than to ask "Why did space travel occur only a few decades ago, even though our brains were preadapted for space travel at least as far back Cro Magnons?". The question ignores the important role of contingency or plain old luck in human evolutionary history.
Thus I regard Rizzolati's discovery — and my purely speculative conjectures on their key role in our evolution — as the most important unreported story of the last decade.
April 24, 2001Science's Elusive Realm: Life's Little Mysteriesby Sandra Blakeslee
SANTA FE, N.M. — Physicists observe the natural world and extract from it laws and principles that reliably explain everyday phenomena. At the smallest scale, they use quantum mechanics to predict the behavior of subatomic particles and small molecules. At larger scales, they devise theories to explain magnetism, the conduction of heat and electricity and other phenomena that occur the same way in a wide variety of materials.
Scientists boast that these principles predict how most matter will behave in physical and chemical experiments.
But there is one region that eludes them. That is the region containing matter on a scale of 10 to 1,000 angstroms (an angstrom being one ten-billionth of a meter) — bigger than a simple molecule but smaller than a living cell. This is the realm in which the constituents of cells interact with one another.
It is where proteins fold, charged ions move through cell membranes and messenger molecules read DNA instructions in the cell nucleus. Even the most advanced microscopes can only glimpse this activity, because the energies they use tend to destroy living tissue, said Dr. David Pines, a physicist at the Los Alamos National Laboratory.
At this level, things do not act according to well-described theories of chemistry and physics. Rather, systems this size seem to obey a unique set of rules that cannot be deduced from studying their individual components.
There are too many atoms in the systems to be described by electromagnetism and quantum theories but too few to handle statistically.
This is the realm of "the mesoscale," and scientists like Dr. Pines and Dr. Robert Laughlin, the Nobel laureate from Stanford, are attacking its mysteries.
"I think we'll see some answers, but it will take a generation or two," Dr. Laughlin said in a recent interview. "It won't happen tomorrow."
Work has begun under the auspices of the Institute for Complex Adaptive Matter or ICAM, a new and independent unit of the Los Alamos National Laboratory and the University of California at Berkeley, which administers the lab. Led by Dr. Pines and Dr. Laughlin, the physicists, chemists and biologists of ICAM met here this year to discuss how scientists might try to understand and maybe even design matter that organizes itself into living systems.
Unlike the Santa Fe Institute and other centers that study complexity, ICAM scientists tend to shun computer models and the jargon of complex systems. Nor do they have much faith in efforts to understand life by sequencing genes and looking for similar patterns in different organisms.
Without deeper organizing principles, they say, the mere accumulation and organization of genetic data will not shed light on how life works.
Research on the mesoscale must be based on experiments, Dr. Laughlin said. "It's about making stuff, putting matter into new situations so you may discover something new. Then you do your best to disprove your theory. Physics teaches us that rules dreamt up without the benefit of physical insight are nearly always wrong. Correct rules must be discovered, not invented."
As an example, he cites 19th century physicists who believed light needed some kind of medium, called ether, to spread through the universe. But this wholly fabricated invention was overturned when Albert Einstein accepted at face value experiments that found light travels at a constant speed and "went on to make his astonishing predictions about the dilation of time and the equivalence of mass and energy, both of which have now been verified in countless experiments," Dr. Laughlin said.
Nanotechnology is exploring matter at the mesoscale and holds promise for discovering new principles, Dr. Laughlin said. But until it develops some theories, it will not be able to test new ideas about how living systems are organized. Mathematicians are beginning to make important contributions that help model and understand biological systems, he said, but laboratory experiments still need to guide their thinking.
To start with, ICAM researchers are focusing on one beguiling fact: complex systems can arise out of simple constituents that interact with each other in ways not necessarily obvious.
In Dr. Laughlin's view, life is constructed according to engineering principles or laws that do not change, though they are observed at different scales under different conditions, Dr. Laughlin said. For example, the laws of hydrodynamics — the science that deals with the motions of fluids and the forces acting on solid bodies immersed in fluids — are the same in a wide variety of materials. Do these so-called "protected" laws exist at the mesoscale? No one attending the ICAM meeting claimed to have the answer. But they discussed a variety of ways to find out.
One approach involves studying the way small molecules called amino acids fold themselves up to make functional proteins. When a gene directs messenger RNA to make a protein, it activates machinery that produces a linear chain of amino acids that code for the protein. Scientists used to think that the same linear sequence of amino acids always led to the same protein, said Dr. Peter Wolynes, a chemist at the University of California at San Diego. They also thought that proteins followed more or less the same course when they folded.
But now, Dr. Wolynes said, scientists know that a huge variety of amino acid sequences can fold up to form the same protein. Moreover, the same string of amino acids fol ded differently creates a protein that acts differently in the body. These findings have turned protein folding into one of the most intractable problems in biology. For example, a big protein like myoglobin, the iron-bearing pigment in muscles, can be made by any one of many millions of different amino acid sequences.
What makes a protein follow one of these many folding possibilities to achieve its functional shape? The answer, Dr. Wolynes said, lies in what he and other researchers call "funnels" in the multidimensional landscape of folding possibilities. Like real funnels that force material to flow in one direction, protein funnels are produced when amino acids try many different configurations and are drawn by an interplay of positive and negative forces to flow or fold in one direction.
But the detailed physical interactions that may create these funnels are not well understood.
Dr. James Shapiro, a professor of microbiology at the University of Chicago and an ICAM researcher, said that interactions among components in a system were the keys to understanding the emergence of complex systems.
These interactions include dynamic properties like feedback and checkpoints, at which the system checks to make sure everything is all right, which are seen everywhere in living cells, he said. All kinds of signals inform cells where they are at a given time, where their neighbors are, what they are supposed to do next and how and when to stop, Dr. Shapiro said.
Feedback and control are processes that lead to protected states in the mesoscale, which need to be explored experimentally. Dr. M. Reza Ghadiri, a chemist at the Scripps Research Institute in San Diego has in his laboratory created small systems of organic molecules that faithfully make copies of themselves and use feedback to change their dynamics.
These molecular ecosystems are not life, he said, but they do show emergent properties like the ability to reproduce, form parasites, correct errors and engage in symbiosis.
Dr. Laughlin challenged biologists to double-check some of their classic experiments used to explain how DNA works at a molecular level. For example, many details of accepted theories of how DNA actually makes proteins are "appallingly bad," he said.
"I've just written a paper on this subject, which is considered nutty by many experts and visionary by others," he said. He said it would be published soon in The Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences and added that he welcomed vigorous debate about it.
At Harvard, Dr. George Whitesides is experimenting with magnetite and iron beads to explore how forces of repulsion, attraction and energy dissipation interact to form unpredictable complex patterns. As these simple systems evolve, Dr. Whitesides said, it should be possible to explore the dynamics of interacting particles and perhaps model biological principles.
"We are letting nature tell us what it likes to do," he said. Such experiments have extraordinary implications, Dr. Pines said. Unlike vitalism — a doctrine that says the processes of life are not explicable by the laws of physics and chemistry alone and that life is in some way self-determining — the research into complex adaptive matter says that life is the consequence of molecular interactions.
"If we can discover organizing principles in biology other than evolution, it means we will be able to make living systems in the laboratory," Dr. Pines said. "We can understand how life began."
Subject: Re: QM and consciousnessFrom: apj@IBB.UNESP.BR (Alfredo Pereira)Date: Wed, May 23, 2001 9:27 AM EDTWhen Kandel, Schwartz and Jessell's *Principles of Neural Science*, third edition, the best and most adopted handbook in the field, is opened at pp.209, a quotation from Bertil Hille appears:"Electricity is used to gate channels and channels are used to makeelectricity. However, the nervous system is not primarily an electricaldevice. Most excitable cells ultimately translate their electricalexcitation into another form of activity. As a broad generalization,excitable cells translate their electricity into action by Ca2+ fluxesmodulated by voltage-sensitive Ca2+ channels...(that) serve as the only link to transduce depolarization into all the nonelectrical activities controlled by excitation".So we can see that at one level of organization the brain IS a quantumsystem. Recently evidence has emerged about how this quantum system works. For example, one of the proteins activated by Ca2+ entrance through theneuron membrane (by the NMDA channel) is calmodulin (CaM). A recent study by Wilson and Brunger (Journal of Molecular Biology, 2000, 301, pp. 1237-1265) revealed that:"Calmodulin...can bind specifically to over 100 protein targets in response to a Ca2+ signal. Ca2+-CaM requires a considerable degree of structuralplasticity to accomplish this physiological role...the evidence for disorder at every accessible length-scale in Ca2+-CaM suggests that the proteinoccupies a large number of hierarchically arranged conformational substrates in the crystalline environment and may sample a quasi-continuous spectrum of conformations in solution. Therefore, we propose that the functionallydistinct forms of CaM are less structurally distinct than previouslybelieved, and that the different activities of CaM in response to Ca2+ may result primarily from Ca2+-mediated alterations in the dynamics of theprotein".Are "Ca2+-mediated alterations" processes explained by classical physics? Of course not. Little is known about this kind of "mesoscopic" process, as was made clear in an excellent interview by S. Blakeslee with scientists fromthe new Institute for Complex Adaptive Matter (ICAM), at Los Alamos Lab., UC/Berkeley - see http://www.nytimes.com/2001/04/24/health/24LIFE.htmlMany molecular biologists, who are presumed to study interactions at this level, limit themselves to the old lock-and-key metaphor when referring to molecular binding (effector-protein, protein-substrate and/orprotein-protein), and on the other hand physicists have not frequentlyfocused on this kind of problem. The exception includes a minority ofinspired physicists who have studied quantum computation in biologicalmedia.I'm publishing a review paper with two Brazilian colleagues where weassociate conscious processing with quantum computation and communication based on intra-cellular proteins which have a central role in signaltransduction pathways and have been experimentally well related to cognitive processing (e.g., proteins recently proved to have a central role in the formation of long-term-memories, as calmodulin-sensitive protein kinase II - see "Alpha-CaMKII-dependent plasticity in the cortex is required forpermanent memory", Frankland, O'Brien, Ohno, Kirkwood & Silva, Nature, 17 May 2001, Vol. 411 No. 6835, pp. 223 - 398).The reference of our paper is:A. Freitas da Rocha, A. Pereira Jr., F.A. B. Coutinho (2001)N-methyl-d-aspartate channel and consciousness: from signal coincidencedetection to quantum computing. Progress in Neurobiology, Vol. 64Issue 6, Aug-2001, pp. 555 - 573.The paper can already be obtained from http://www.neuroscion.com/This is a site I reccomend for everyone interested in neuroscience.Registering for a free trial gives "credits" that can be used to "purchase" this and other papers from several journals.Best Regards to all,Alfredo Pereira Jr.Professor of Philosophy of ScienceState University of Sao Paulo (UNESP)Botucatu - Sao Paulo - Brasil
THE BIOLOGICAL FUNCTION OF THE THIRD EYEby Richard Alan Miller, ©1975.
From the time of Dionysius to the time of Plato, the cultures of the Mediterranean consented to the doctrine that claimed the existence of an order of ultimate reality which lies beyond apparent reality. This "paranormal" reality was accessible to the consciousness only when the "normal" routines of mental data processing were dislocated. It was Plato's pupil Aristotle who changed his teacher's game, separating physics from metaphysics. The philosophical temper of our present civilization, being scientifically and technically oriented, is basically Aristotelian.
No such rational figure as Aristotle arose in the Orient to a position of equal eminence. Because of this and other reasons, Indian anatomists and zoologists, who where no doubt just as curious as the Greeks about the origins of life, and as skilled in dissection, did not feel compelled to set their disciplines up in opposition to metaphysics. Physical and metaphysical philosophy remained joined like Siamese twins. As a result, the discipline which became medicine in the West evolved into a system known as Kundalini Yoga in the Hindu culture.
In Western terms, Kundalini Yoga can be best understood as a biological statement contained within the language of the poetic metaphor. The system makes the attempt of joining the seeming disparate entities of body and mind. It is a very complicated doctrine; in oversimplified terms, the system encourages the practitioner to progress through the control of a number of stages, called Chakras or mind-body coordination. A sixth, associated with clairvoyance and telepathy, is called the Ajna.
The physiological site of this sixth Chakra, the Ajna, is located in the center of the forehead. It is symbolized by an eye - the so-called third eye, the inner eye, or the eye of the mind. When this eye is opened, a new and completely different dimension of reality is revealed to the practitioner of yoga. Western scholars when they first encountered this literature, took the third eye to be an appropriately poetic metaphor and nothing else.
It was not until the middle of the nineteenth century, as the subcontinent of Australia and its surrounding territory came to be explored, that a flurry of interest centered upon a lizard native to the area, the tuatara (Sphenodon punctatum). This animal possessed, in addition to two perfectly ordinary eyes located on either side of its head, a third eye buried in the skull which was revealed through an aperture in the bone, covered by a transparent membrane, and surrounded by a rosette of scales. It was unmistakably a third eye but upon dissection it proved to be non-functional.
Though this eye still possessed the structure of a lens and a retina, these were found to be no longer in good working order: also lacking were the appropriate neural connections to the brain. The presence of this eye in the tuatara still posses a puzzle to present-day evolutionists, for almost all vertebrates possess a homologous structure in the center of their skull. It is present in many fish, all reptiles, birds, and mammals (including man). This structure is known in literature today as the pineal gland.
The gland is shaped like a tine pine cone situated deep in the middle of the brain between the two hemispheres. Studies then began to determine whether this organ was a true functioning gland or merely a vestigial sight organ, a relic from our reptilian past. In 1959 Dr. Aaron Lerner and his associates at Yale University found that meletonin (1), a hormone manufactured by the pineal gland, was created through the action of certain enzymes on a precursor chemical which must pre-exist in the pineal in order for it to be transformed into melatonin. This precursor chemical turned out to be serotonin (2).
It was E.J. Gaddum, a professor of pharmacology at the University of Edinburgh, who was the first to note a connection between serotonin and mental states of being. In a paper published in 1953, he pointed out the fact that LSD-25 was a potent antagonist to serotonin. Serotonin is not an unusual chemical in nature; it is found in many places - some of them odd, like the salivary glands of octopuses, others ordinary; it abounds in plants such as bananas, figs, and plums. What then is its function in the human brain?
The task of exploring the role played by melatonin, and its precursor serotonin, was undertaken by a biochemist, Julius Axelrod. He found that melatonin suppressed physiological sexuality in mammals. If test animals were stimulated to manufacture excessive amounts of melatonin, their gonads and ovaries tended to become reduced in size, to shrink, to atrophy. The estrous or fertility cycle in females could likewise be altered experimentally by doses of melatonin.
Now two most curious functions had been attributed to the pineal gland, the third eye of the mind:
(1) It has now been established that this organ produced a chemical which had, indirectly at least, been associated with psychedelic states. The chemical substance melanin is the pigment which darkens skin color. It is located in specialized cells scattered through the topmost layer of skin. Melatonin was found to be the substance responsible for causing the contraction of melanin-producing cells.
(2) It also produced a chemical which suppressed functional sexuality. Serotonin is of the same chemical series of indole alkaloids which include psychedelic drugs such as LSD-25, psilocybin, D.M.T. and bufotenine. The hormone serotonin is also known as 5-hydroxtryptamine.
The literature of religious mysticism in all ages and all societies has viewed the mystical passion of ecstasy as being somehow antagonistic to, or in competition with, carnal passion.
Axelrod and his co-workers also discovered another incredible fact: the pineal gland produces its chemical according to a regular oscillating beat, the basis of this beat being the so-called circadian rhythm. They found that the pineal responded somehow to light conditions, that by altering light conditions they could extend, contract, or even stabilize the chemical production rhythms of the pineal.
The fact that the pineal responds to light, even if this response is indirect via the central nervous system, has some fascinating and far-reaching conceptual applications. There are many behavioral changes which overtake animals as the seasons change, and which can be produced out of season in the laboratory by simulating the appropriate span of artificial daylight. Do such seasonal changes in mood and behavior persist in humans?
The great religious holy days of all faiths tend to cluster around the times of the solstices and equinoxes. Is it possible that the human pineal gland responds to these alterations in length of daylight? Changing the balance of neurohumors in the brain may perhaps effect a greater incidence of psychedelic states in certain susceptible individuals just at these crucial times. This possibility provides an entirely new potential dimension to our secular understanding of the religious experience.
The pineal gland has thus been referred to as a kind of biological clock, one which acts as a kind of coupling system; perhaps maintaining phase relations within a multi-oscillator system; a phase coordinator for multiple bio-rhythms. The pineal is a "cosmic eye;" it is aware of celestial rhythm. It "tunes" our biochemistry to those subtle rhythms not observed by the normal eye, like seasonal and lunar changes rather than daily ones. Serotonin can be seen as the "intensity knob" of the brain. As the level of serotonin increases, so does the level of activation of the cortex.
Strong suspicion has fallen now on serotonin as being one of the principle agents of the psychedelic experience. Studies now reveal that LSD-25 strikes like a chemical guerrilla, entering into receptor granules in the brain cells swiftly, and then leaving after a very short time, perhaps ten to twenty minutes (in animals). When the bulk of LSD-25 has left the receptor granules, it is replaced by what seems to be excessive, or super-normal amounts of serotonin. The LSD-25 creates what is called a "bouncing effect," like a spring pushed too tight. When the LSD-25 leaves the system, the serotonin springs back and overcompensates.
For most of us, most of the time, our world is a Darwinian environment. We must manipulate ourselves within it, or attempt to manipulate it in order to survive. These survival needs tend to color our appreciation of this world, and we are continually making judgments about it. Some of these judgments are based on prior personal experience, others are provide by the culture. This "recognition system" is one of the elements disrupted by the psychedelic state.
The principle question concerning psychedelic states remains: How much disruption can the system tolerate? The problem of how to maintain a certain madness while at the same time functioning at peak efficiency has now captured the attention of many psychiatrists. There seems to be a point at which Edgar Allen Poe's "creative madness" becomes degenerative, impeding function rather than stimulating it.
In light of this analysis, a shaman can be seen to be uncoupling his internal bio-sensor from the universal inputs. He gets "drift" where he is rushed toward new signal-to-noise ratios. The particular rituals are set up to disconnect the shaman from his social and cosmic environment. This is done through the ritual use of hallucinogens; they de-synchronize his internal rhythms. This de-synchronization produces more noise in his awareness. It also expands that awareness. The rituals are so designed as to contain elements which focus or tune that "noise" and direct the expanded awareness.
Man is unique by virtue of being possessed by intuitions concerning the scope of the mysterious universe he inhabits. He has devised for himself all manner of instruments to prove the nature of this universe. The beginnings of scientific understanding of shamanistic ritual and the function of the third eye provide man with powerful new techniques for exploration. This will allow him to penetrate the vast interior spaces where the history of millions of years of memories lies entangled among the roots of the primordial self.
INTRODUCTION TO TRANSCRANIAL MAGNETIC STIMULATION
SUMMARY: Brain stimulation with TMS is achieved from the outside of the head using pulses of electromagnetic field that induce an electric field in the brain. TMS has numerous applications in the study, diagnosis and therapy of the brain. TMS can either excite the cortex or disturb its function. The concurrent use of TMS and high-resolution EEG shows that the combination is effective for mapping the functional connections in the brain. Under EEG, a TMS pulse to the motor area of the left hemisphere is seen to move to the opposite hemisphere, suggesting a callosal connection between the two active areas. The neuronal response to magnetic stimulation reveals cortical reactivity and connectivity.
TMS holds special promise as a tool to study localization of function, connectivity of brain regions, and pathophysiology of neuropsychiatric disorders. It may also have potential as a therapeutic intervention. TMS has been referred to as "electrodeless" electrical stimulation, to emphasize that the magnetic field acts as the medium between electricity in the coil and induced electrical currents in the brain. The proximity of the brain to the time-varying magnetic field results in current flow in neural tissues.
Neuronal depolarization can also be produced by electrical stimulation, with electrodes placed on the scalp (referred to as transcranial electric stimulation). Electroconvulsive therapy (ECT) is an example of this. Importantly, unlike electrical stimulation, where the skull acts as a massive resistor, magnetic fields are not deflected or attenuated by intervening tissue. This means that TMS can be more focal than electric stimulation. Furthermore, for electrical stimulation to achieve sufficient current density in brain to result in neuronal depolarization, pain receptors in the scalp must be stimulated.
A striking effect of TMS occurs when one places the coil on the scalp over primary motor cortex. A single TMS pulse of sufficient intensity causes involuntary movement. The magnetic field intensity needed to produce motor movement varies considerably across individuals, and is known as the motor threshold. Placing the coil over different areas of the motor cortex causes contralateral movement in different distal muscles, corresponding to the well-known homunculus. Transcranial magnetic stimulation can be used to map the representation of body parts in the motor cortex on an individual basis. Subjectively, this stimulation feels much like a tendon reflex movement.
Thus, a TMS pulse produces a powerful but brief magnetic field that passes through the skin, soft tissue, and skull, and induces electrical current in neurons, causing depolarization that then has behavioral effects (body movement). The TMS magnetic field declines logarithmically with distance from the coil. This limits the area of depolarization with current technology to a depth of about 2 cm below the brain's surface.
Single TMS over motor cortex can produce simple movements. Over primary visual cortex, TMS can produce the perception of flashes of light or phosphenes. To date, these are the "positive" behavioral effects of TMS. Other immediate behavioral effects are generally disruptive. Interference with information processing and behavior is especially likely when TMS pulses are delivered rapidly and repetitively. Repeated rhythmic TMS is called repetitive TMS (rTMS). If the stimulation occurs faster than once per second (1 Hz) it is referred to as fast rTMS.
A key distinction between TMS research and work on the behavioral effects of exposure to magnetic fields is that TMS effects occur at or near intensities sufficient to produce cortical neuron depolarization. The capacity to noninvasively excite or inhibit focal cortical areas represents a remarkable advance for neuroscience research. As an interventional probe in neuropsychiatric disorders, rTMS has the potential of taking functional imaging one step further by elucidating causal relationships. Experimental treatment of depression with TMS showed evidence that modulation of precrontal function is linked to the efficacy of ECT. Studies combining SSRIs with rTMS showed the rTMS group with a faster antidepressant response. It is unknown whether the effect is region or frequency dependent. TMS is relatively benign. Repetetive TMS does not involve anesthesia administration or seizure induction and has no ovious sequelae as does ECT.
There is evidence that rTMS can modulate mood systems in normal volunteers. Three studies found that rTMS over the left DLPFC transiently induced a mild increase in self-rated sadness, whereas right DLPFC rTMS produced a mild increase in self-rated happiness as early as 20 minutes or as late as 5 to 8 hours poststimulation. As described, the mood effects of rTMS in patients with major depression may have an opposite laterality to those seen in normal volunteers. There has yet to be an investigation using TMS to probe the anatomy subserving the perception or expression of emotion
Transcranial magnetic stimulation carries the vision of tailoring the site and nature of stimulation to individual needs. It is uncertain whether this vision will be realized and whether a treatment role for rTMS will emerge. At the practical level, rTMS research is not supported with the resources devoted to pharmaceutical development. Given the large parameter space, it is difficult to see how rTMS treatment applications can be optimized without considerable basic research extending from cell culture preparations through whole animal models, including humans.
Stimulation of one hemisphere can inhibit or facilitate responses elicited in the opposite hemisphere, indicating interhemispheric modulatory effects. Paired-pulse inhibition is reduced in focal epilepsy and enhanced by -aminobutryic acid (GABA)-ergic agents. Pharmacological manipulations suggest that intracortical paired-pulse inhibition reflects the activation of inhibitory GABA-ergic and dopaminergic interneurons, while paired-pulse facilitation reflects excitatory N-methyl-D-aspartate-mediated interneurons, and motor threshold is modulated by ion channel conductivity. These profiles provide novel methods to investigate local alterations in neurochemical systems.
Some preliminary studies suggest that rTMS effects on cortical excitability may depend on the frequency of stimulation. Manipulations of frequency and intensity may produce distinct patterns of facilitation (fast rTMS) and inhibition (slow rTMS) of motor responses with distinct time courses. These effects may last beyond the duration of the rTMS trains with enduring effects on spontaneous neuronal firing rates.
To use TMS optimally, it is important to know how TMS is acting in the brain. Does TMS mimic normal brain physiology, or is it supraphysiologically depolarizing and activating different cell groups (excitatory, inhibitory, local, or remote) in a large region? Understanding of TMS mechanisms is being advanced through studies in animal models and by combining TMS with functional neuroimaging.
Neuroimaging studies have shown that TMS is biologically active, both locally in tissue under the coil and at remote sites, presumably through transsynaptic connections. Several studies have shown that the different parameters used in rTMS (location, intensity, frequency) affect the extent and type of neurophysiological alterations. Thus, there is considerable promise that functional imaging research will help elucidate basic TMS effects and the roles that different TMS parameters exert in modulating these effects. Theoretically, this may advance clinical research, particularly if combinations of location, intensity, and frequency are found to have divergent effects on neuronal activity. Transcranial magnetic stimulation imaging studies can be divided into 2 main categories: (1) using imaging to guide TMS coil placement and understand the spatial distribution of TMS magnetic fields in the brain, and (2) using imaging to measure TMS effects on neuronal activity.
Bohning et al demonstrated that an MRI scanner can be used to display the TMS magnetic field (producing a phase map; Figure 2). This work confirmed that the TMS field is not altered appreciably by head geometry. Further, by combining several TMS coils with different relative orientations, this technique can measure in 3 dimensions the capacity to focus and combine magnetic fields. Ultimately, TMS coil arrays combined with MRI may target deep brain structures. Owing to seizure risk at moderate intensity, fast rTMS can only be given in short pulse trains (1-8 seconds) with relatively long intervals between trains (20 seconds).
A major hypothesis in the TMS field has been that fast rTMS results in excitatory physiological changes, while slow rTMS has inhibitory effects. To date, imaging studies have yielded inconsistent results regarding this proposition. In fact, some slow rTMS imaging studies over motor or prefrontal cortex have found decreased local and remote brain activity, while others have found increases. Some imaging studies of fast rTMS have found increased perfusion, but not all.
Transcranial magnetic stimulation is not pleasant, and stimulation at higher intensities and frequencies is generally more painful. The pain experienced during rTMS is likely related to the repetitive stimulation of peripheral facial and scalp muscles, resulting in muscle tension headaches in a proportion of subjects (approximately 5%-20% depending on the study). These headaches respond to treatment with acetaminophen or aspirin. Magnetic stimulation also produces a high-frequency noise artifact that can cause short-term changes in hearing threshold. This is avoided when subjects and investigators wear earplugs.
rTMS has resulted in seizures. The risk of seizure induction is related to the parameters of stimulation, and no seizures have been reported with single-pulse TMS or rTMS delivered at a slow frequency (<1 Hz). There is a growing understanding of the rTMS parameter combinations (magnetic intensity, pulse frequency, train duration, and intertrain interval) that result in spread of excitation, heralding impending seizure. Even if therapeutic benefits are convincingly shown, the seizure risk may limit the widespread and loosely supervised use of rTMS. In part for this reason, the therapeutic potential of slow-frequency (<1 Hz) deserves particular attention.
Gates et al performed histological examinations of the resected temporal lobes of 2 patients with epilepsy who preoperatively received approximately 2000 stimulations over this tissue.[143] Lesions attributable to TMS were not found. Magnetic resonance imaging scans done before and after 2 weeks of rTMS in 30 depressed patients did not show change.
Both TMS and rTMS can disrupt cognition during the period of stimulation. However, the safety concerns are about alterations in cognitive function beyond the period of stimulation. The limited investigation of short-term neuropsychological effects of TMS has not demonstrated significant changes.[39] Little information is available about long-term effects. The technique has been in use for more than a decade without reports of long-term adverse consequences. The rate of cancer is not increased in individuals with prolonged exposure to high-intensity magnetic fields, such as MRI technicians. However, TMS involves extremely brief, focal exposure to high-intensity magnetic fields and thus safety information from MRI technicians, or even people who live near power lines (lengthy exposure to low-intensity magnetic fields) may not be germane.
Controlled trials across a variety of neuropsychiatric conditions are underway, yet safety information is limited. Reassuringly, single-pulse and other TMS measures of cortical excitability are believed to be devoid of significant safety concerns. However, rTMS has shown potential to ameliorate neuropsychiatric symptoms. The potential for adverse cognitive effects must be considered precisely because it is hypothesized that rTMS is a sufficiently powerful modulator of regional functional activity to have therapeutic properties. More comprehensive neuropsychological evaluations of the short- and long-term effects of rTMS are needed.
ECT presents the one situation in humans in which seizures are provoked for therapeutic purposes. A reliable method of seizure induction with TMS may have important advantages over traditional ECT by offering better control over the intensity and spatial distribution of current density in the brain. Developing a TMS form of convulsive therapy is largely an issue of technological advances in stimulator output and coil design. Such a development may also foster better understanding of the safety of nonconvulsive uses of rTMS.
CONCLUSIONS
During the next several years, it will become clearer whether rTMS has a role in the treatment of psychiatric disorders. To date, trials in depression have focused on demonstrating antidepressant properties and have not demonstrated clinical utility. We need to know a good deal more about the patients who benefit from rTMS, the optimal form of treatment delivery, the magnitude and persistence of therapeutic effects, the capability of sustaining improvement with rTMS or other modalities, and the risks of treatment. It is still too early to know whether we are at the threshold of a new era in physical treatments and noninvasive regional brain modulation. Regardless of its potential therapeutic role, the capacity of rTMS to noninvasively and focally alter functional brain activity should lead to important advances in our understanding of brain-behavior relationships and the pathophysiology of neuropsychiatric disorders.
-------------------------------
List of abbreviationsCT Computed tomography EEG Electroencephalography EMG Electromyography EP Evoked potential ERP Event-related potential ES Electrical stimulation FEM Finite element method fMRI Functional magnetic resonance imaging MEG Magnetoencephalography MEP Motor-evoked potential MNE Minimum-norm estimation MRI Magnetic resonance imaging MT Motor threshold NIRS Near-infrared spectroscopy PET Positron emission tomography PNS Peripheral nervous system rTMS Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation SPECT Single photon emission computed tomography TCES Transcranial electrical stimulation TMS Transcranial magnetic stimulation 3D Three-dimensional
----------------------
Appendixes
Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation: Modelling and New TechniquesJarmo Ruohonen (Finland), 1998
1 Introduction
The use of non-invasive neuroimaging has increased explosively in recent years. Details of the functioning of the human brain are revealed by measuring electromagnetic fields outside the head or metabolic and hemodynamic changes using electroencephalography (EEG), magnetoencephalography (MEG), positron emission tomography (PET), near-infrared spectroscopy (NIRS) or functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). This thesis deals with transcranial magnetic brain stimulation (TMS), which is a direct way of manipulating and interfering with the function of the cortex, thus complementing conventional neuroimaging.
Brain stimulation with TMS is achieved from the outside of the head using pulses of electromagnetic field that induce an electric field in the brain. TMS has numerous applications in the study, diagnosis and therapy of the brain. TMS can either excite the cortex or disturb its function. The observed excitatory effects are normally muscle twitches or phosphenes, whereas in the "lesion" mode TMS can transiently suppress perception or interfere with task performance.
The aim of this thesis was to develop physical understanding of magnetic stimulation and to build models that could provide new insights for utilising the technique. For this purpose, two principal issues had to be addressed: 1) macroscopic electromagnetic fields in the tissue, for which models are developed in Publications I-III, and 2) understanding of the neuronal responses, considered in Publications IV and V. Then, the models developed were used as a basis for engineering modifications that would increase the utility of TMS, the emphasis being on the optimisation of the stimulating coils (Publication VI) and on the use of multiple coils in a whole-scalp array (Publication VII). Publication VIII presents the concurrent use of TMS and high-resolution EEG, showing that the combination is effective for mapping the functional connections in the brain.
The models and procedures were developed in parallel with the design and construction of TMS instrumentation for computer-assisted stimulation.
2.1 Basic principles
Neurones can be excited by externally applied time-varying electromagnetic fields. In TMS, excitation is achieved by driving intense pulses of current I(t) through a coil located above the head. The source of activation is the electric field E induced in the tissue, obtained from Faraday’s law:
where B is the magnetic field produced by the coil, given by the Biot-Savart law:
The integration is performed with the vector dl along the coil windings C and m0= 4p´ 10-7 H/m is the permeability of free space.
The pulses of current are generated with a circuit containing a discharge capacitor connected with the coil in series by a thyristor. With the capacitor first charged to 2-3 kV, the gating of the thyristor into the conducting state will cause the discharging of the capacitor through the coil. The resulting current waveform is typically a damped sinusoidal pulse that lasts about 300 ms and has a peak value of 5-10 kA. The electrical principles have been outlined, e.g., by Jalinous [72,73].
Figure 1 summarises the chain of events in TMS. The induced E is strongest near the coil and typically stimulates a cortical area of a few centimetres in diameter. TMS pulses cause coherent firing of neurones in the stimulated area as well as changed firing due to synaptic input. At microscopic level, E affects the neurones’ transmembrane voltage and thereby the voltage-sensitive ion channels. Brain imaging tools can be used to detect the associated electrical currents and changes in blood flow of metabolism. In motor-cortex stimulation, peripheral effects can be observed as muscle activity with surface electromyography (EMG). Moreover, there may be behavioural changes, for instance, impaired task performance.
Stimulation of the exposed human cerebral cortex with electrical currents was first described by Bartholow in 1874 [11]; the currents elicited movements of the opposite side of the body. Electrical brain stimulation is today possible non-invasively using scalp electrodes [96]. However, transcranial electrical stimulation (TCES) is very painful and hence of limited value.
The first experiments with magnetic stimulation were conducted by d’Arsonval in 1896 [36]. He reported "phosphenes and vertigo, and in some persons, syncope," when the subject's head was placed inside an induction coil. Later, many scientists reported the phenomenon of magnetophosphenes, that is, visual sensations caused by the stimulation of the retina due to changing magnetic fields [10,15,41,92,155,159].
Magnetic nerve stimulation was accomplished only several decades later, first in the frog by Kolin et al. [79] in 1959 and then in the human peripheral nerve by Bickford and Fremming [17] in 1965. The latter authors used an oscillatory magnetic field that lasted 40 ms. The resulting long-lasting activation interval made it impossible to record nerve or muscle action potentials, and the work was not pursued further. In the following years, the technique was investigated only occasionally [68,87,118].
In 1982, Polson, Barker and Freeston [128] described a prototype magnetic stimulator for peripheral nerve stimulation. They used 2-ms-duration pulses and recorded, for the first time, motor-evoked potentials (MEPs) obtained by median nerve magnetic stimulation. In present-day devices, the pulse duration is typically shorter.
In 1985, the Sheffield group achieved successful transcranial magnetic stimulation [9] and made the first clinical examinations [6]. TMS proved valuable for probing the motor pathways: in healthy subjects, stimulation over the motor cortex causes twitches in hand muscles in about 25 ms, while many neurological conditions manifest slower conduction. Another important characteristic of TMS is that it is painless, the subject usually feeling only a not uncomfortable sensation of scalp being pinched. The encouraging results led into commercialisation of TMS by Novametrix Ltd. (predecessor of Magstim Company).
Since 1985, magnetic stimulator technology has remained mostly unchanged. Whereas early research used circular coils, today devices are usually equipped also with an 8-shaped, or figure-of-eight coil proposed by Ueno [157]. The 8-shaped coil induces a more concentrated electric field than the circular coil, resulting in better control of the spatial extent of the excitation. Another important development is repetitive TMS (rTMS) capable of delivering trains of stimuli at 1-50 Hz. rTMS was first produced by Cadwell Laboratories in 1988 and is today one of the most quickly growing areas of TMS research.
Magnetic therapy or rapid transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) has been proposed as a possible new treatment for severe depression. This treatment involves the passage of magnetic waves through the skull using a special machine. Several treatments are required. Unlike electro-convulsive therapy, no anesthesia is necessary. There are no obvious major side effects from treatment.
From a medical perspective, the use of rTMS is based on studies which suggest that the left prefrontal lobe of the brain is pathophysiologically linked to depression. When research was conducted using non depressed volunteers, it showed that rTMS to prefrontal structure in the brain had a lateralised effect on mood. When preliminary studies were conducted using volunteers with depression a beneficial effect of rTMS on depression was suggested.
This treatment is in its very early stages and the limited research findings suggest that there may be improvement in selected patients. To date, the treatment has only been used in patients with very severe depression who have failed all other treatments. Therefore, at this point in time, magnetic therapy is mostly applied to patients who have severely resistant depression. As the role and effectiveness of this treatment become better established, its possible use across a broad range of depressive illnesses can be better evaluated. At that time, its role in the treatment of major depression as compared with anti-depressants and particularly electro-convulsive therapy can be further evaluated.
-------------------------------------------
April 1999
Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation
Applications in NeuropsychiatryMark S. George, MD; Sarah H. Lisanby, MD; Harold A. Sackeim, PhD
In the 1990s, it is difficult to open a newspaper or watch television and not find someone claiming that magnets promote healing. Rarely do these claims stem from double-blind, peer-reviewed studies, making it difficult to separate the wheat from the chaff. The current fads resemble those at the end of the last century, when many were falsely touting the benefits of direct electrical and weak magnetic stimulation. Yet in the midst of this popular interest in magnetic therapy, a new neuroscience field has developed that uses powerful magnetic fields to alter brain activity—transcranial magnetic stimulation. This review examines the basic principles underlying transcranial magnetic stimulation, and describes how it differs from electrical stimulation or other uses of magnets. Initial studies in this field are critically summarized, particularly as they pertain to the pathophysiology and treatment of neuropsychiatric disorders. Transcranial magnetic stimulation is a promising new research and, perhaps, therapeutic tool, but more work remains before it can be fully integrated in psychiatry's diagnostic and therapeutic armamentarium.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1999;56:300-311
-------------------------------------------------------
Bioelectricity in Neurons
Misconceptions about neurobio-electrical energy
Many encyclopedias, dictionaries, and textbooks contain very clear statements about the nature ofbio-electricity. They say this:
Bio-electricity is a type of energy. Bio-Electric current is a flow of energy.
The above statements are wrong. Bio-electricity is not energy. Bio-electricity and bio-electrical energy are two different things. It's not too difficult to show that this is true. Below is a collection of simple facts which demonstrate that bio-electricity, the stuff that flows within neural axons/dendrites, is not a form of energy.
In a simple neuro-electric circuit, the bio-electricity flows in a circle. No bio-electricity is ever gained or lost. On the other hand, the neuron is a battery (replenished by ADP Na+/K+ pump) that creates bio-electrical energy.
In a neuron, bio-electricity flows through the body and none is lost. Bio-electricity enters the neuron through dendritic wires, and the same amount leaves through the axonal wire. Yet the neuron uses up bio-electric energy: the bio-electric energy flows into the neuron, and it does not come back out again.
In a neuron, the bio-electricity (Na, K) moves back and forth across the leaky membranes. In other words, the charged particles remain localized, they simply oscillate back and forth across a local membrane. Ions do not move forward at all (if they did, that would be a direct current or "DC.") At the same time, the bio-electrical energy moves forward rapidly. Only the bio-electricity "vibrates." The energy does not; the bio-energy flows continuously forwards.
Ionic charges are not in and of themselves energy any more than the water particles in the above energy wave.
The neuronal action potential therefore is a way of transmitting neuroelectrical energy without transmitting the ionic particles of electricity (Na, K, Ca, etc).
My above statements about bio-electricity would be accepted by most scientists throughout history, including Ben Franklin, Michael Faraday, James C. Maxwell and Robert Millikan. I'm using the word bio-electricity in the same manner as they did: bio-electricity is the positive and negative "stuff" that's found in electrons and protons. It is the "substance" that flows along inside of the wires. These scientists would call this flow a "current of bio-electricity." They'd say that any charged object has a "charge of bio-electricity," and that anions and cations are "particles of bio-electricity."
MORE TRUE STATEMENTS ABOUT "bio-electricity"
If we know the precise amount of bio-electricity flowing per second through a wire (the Amperes,) this tells us nothing about the amount of energy being delivered per second (the Watts.) A bio-electric current is not a flow of energy.
In a bio-electric circuit, the flow of the bio-electricity is measured in Coulombs per second (Amperes.) The flow of energy is measured in Joules per second (Watts.) A Coulomb is not a Joule, and there is no way to convert from Coulombs to Joules or from Amperes to Watts. A flow of bio-electricity is not a flow of energy.
In a DC circuit, the bio-electricity within the wires flows exceedingly slowly; on the order of inches per minute. At the same time, the electrical energy flows at nearly the speed of light.
Bio-Electrical energy is bio-electromagnetism; it is a bio-electromagnetic field. The particles of bio-electricity (anions/cations) flowing within a wire have little resemblance to an electromagnetic field. They are matter. Bio-electricity is not energy, instead it is a component of everyday matter.
In a neuro-electric circuit, bio-electrical energy does not flow inside the axon. Instead it flows in the space surrounding the axons. Physics books describe one method of measuring this flow: take the vector cross-product of the E and M components of the electromagnetic field at all points in a plane penetrated by the wires. We call this the Poynting Vector field. Add these measurements together, and this tells us the total energy flow (the Joules of energy that flow each second.) To discover the rate of energy flow, don't look at the flowing electrons. The bio-electricity flow tells us little. Instead look at the electromagnetic fields which surround the wires.
In any bio-electric circuit, the smallest particle of electrical energy is NOT the anion/cation, the electron or proton. The smallest particle of energy is the unit quantum of electromagnetic energy: it is the bio-photon. Bio-electricity is made of ions, while bio-electrical energy is made of photons.
In the bio-electric neuronal ensemble, a certain amount of energy is lost because it flies off into extra-cellular space. This is well understood: electrical energy is electromagnetic waves, and unless the axons are somehow shielded, (which they are with Schwann cells) they will act as 60Hz antennas. Waves of 60Hz electrical energy will spread outwards into space rather than following the wires.
A battery or generator is like your heart: it moves blood, but it does not create blood. When a generator stops, or when the a metal circuit is opened, all the electrons stop where they are, and the wires remain filled with electric charges. But this isn't unexpected, because the wires were full of vast quantities of charge in the first place.
Neuro-electric currents in axons are a flow of ions, but these ions are not supplied by neurons themselves. They come from the both the intra and extra cellular ions. These ions were already in the circuit before the ATP-battery was connected. Batteries and generators do not create these ions, they merely pump them, and the ions are like a pre-existing fluid that is always found within all axons. In order to understand neuro-electric circuits, we must imagine that all the axons are pre-filled with a sort of "liquid electricity."
A bio-electric current is a FLOW OF CHARGE. A bio-electric current is a flowing motion of charged particles, anions/cations. The words "Electric Current" mean the same as "charge flow." Bio-electric current is a very slow flow of charges. On the other hand, electric energy is made of fields and it moves VERY rapidly. Neuro-electric energy moves at a different speed than neuro-electric current, so obviously they are two different things.
Neuro-electric energy is composed of electric and magnetic fields, and it exists in the space surrounding the axons. Neuro-electric energy is very similar to radio waves, but it is very low in frequency. Neuro-electric CHARGE is very different than the energy. The charge-flow (current) is a flowing motion usually of ions, and ions are material particles, not energy particles.
And it's not always a flow of electrons: when electric current exists inside an electrolyte (in batteries, salt water, the earth, or in your flesh) it is a flow of charged atoms called ions, so there is no denying that it is a flow of material. Current is a matter-flow, not an energy flow.
The energy in neuro-electric circuits is not carried by individual ions, it is carried by the circuit as a whole.
Here's one way to clarify the muddled concepts: if electric current is like a flow of air inside a pipe, then electrical energy is like sound waves in the pipe, and electrons are like the air molecules. Sound can travel through a pipe if the pipe is full of air molecules, and electrical energy can flow along a wire because the wire is full of movable charges. Sound moves much faster than wind, correct? And electrical energy moves much faster than electric current for much the same reason.
In any neuro-electric circuit, the smallest particle of electrical energy is NOT the anion/cation or electron/proton. The smallest particle of energy is the unit quantum of electromagnetic energy: it is the photon. Neuro-electricity and its currents are made of ions, while electrical energy is made of photons.
Just as in modern electronics, a radio for instance, information is not carried in the movement of electrons in the wires and antennas but in the modulations of electro-magnetic waves, neural information is not carried by ionic currents, action potentials, but in the modulation of bio-electromagnetic energy - ie. bio-photons.
A photon is not simply a quantum of visible light, but a quantum of the entire electromagnetic spectrum of energy. Since neurological systems produce electromagnetic energy, bio-photons should not be a surprise.
Tuesday, May 8, 2007
The Future of Mind Science
Mind-scientists have their creeds, just as religious
believers do. Francis Crick spelled out his reductionist
vision at the beginning of his book “The Astonishing
Hypothesis.”
“You: your joys and your sorrows, your memories
and your ambitions, your sense of personal identity
and free will, are in fact no more than the behavior
of a vast assembly of nerve cells and their associated
molecules.” As Lewis Carroll’s Alice might have
phrased it, “You’re nothing but a pack of neurons.”
In a sense, Crick is right. We are nothing but a
pack of neurons. At the same time, neuroscience has
so far proved to be oddly unsatisfactory. Explaining
the mind in terms of neurons has not yielded much
more insight or benefit than explaining the mind in
terms of quarks and electrons.
There are many alternative reductionisms. We are
nothing but a pack of idiosyncratic genes. We are nothing
but a pack of adaptations sculpted by natural selection.
We are nothing but a pack of computational
devices dedicated to different tasks. We are nothing
but a pack of sexual neuroses. These proclamations,
like Crick’s, are all defensible, and they are all inadequate....
Read More
Monday, May 7, 2007
Wednesday, May 2, 2007
Was Timothy Leary Right?
Read the whole article there
Video Log
Audio-Visual Performance Xperiments : Part A Audio-visual jockey from ikar on Vimeo.
Teratone Vision Audio-Video workshop @ trip bubble lab.
::AV Mixing
Edirol V4 Video Mixer + Gemini DJ Audio Mixer
Alternate solo configuration with AV Mixer Demo
::AV Vinyl Timeline Control, Pitch & Scratch
Technics MkII Turntables + MsPinky IWS
::AV FX's
Legacy kaos pad with linked Audio & Video mapping
VJ Ikar + VJ Soyouth
Sony & Dell Dualcore laptops, Edirol & M-Audio Asio Soundcards, Novation Remote25 Controller
Expect some upgrade in the months to come ;-)
teratone.org